Sunday, April 3, 2011

Global Concerns and Predictions

There are many global concerns and predictions mentioned in the chapters I made my posts on, but two of them especially stood out to me.  These two ideas include: the globalization of culture and the concern of bio and cyber-terrorism.  These concepts stood out to me not only because of how relevant they are to the world today, but also because I think it is extremely important to understand and know about these kinds of issues.
The Globalization of culture happens when a single culture starts to diffuse throughout the world and begin to mix with other cultures.  This has been taking place for a while now, but according to Margaret Anderson and Howard Taylor, "The infusion of Western culture throughout the world seems to be accelerating as the commercialized culture of the United States is marketed worldwide."  In other words, because of the influence of Capitalist markets, U.S. culture can be seen in all different countries all over the world.  For example you can travel to a place as far away as Europe or Asia and still be able to go to McDonald's for lunch.  Other industries including fashion and entertainment are also a part of the diffusion of culture that is taking place.  Because of this, the older cultures and traditions that are found throughout the world are being pushed aside while Western culture is forcing its way in.  The problem with this is the potential depletion of these wonderful, diverse cultures and traditions; however, there is also a positive side.
"Global economic change can also introduce more tolerant values to cultures that might have had a narrower worldview previously" (Anderson, Taylor 41).  In other words, cultures that may have had a more narrow view of the world and of other cultures may become more tolerant and accepting of them because of this change.
The second global concern I found very relevant and interesting I read about in chapter seven and is the increased fear of new kinds of terrorism. These include bioterrorism and cyberterrorism.  In the past, countries have experienced many forms of terrorism including hijackings, bombings, suicide attacks and so on, but today we have more to worry about.  Bioterrorism is "the form of terrorism involving the dispersion of chemical or biological substances intended to cause widespread disease and death" (Anderson, Taylor 175).  This form of terrorism could potentially kill millions of people in a very short amount of time by using viruses or disease causing bacteria to infect a population.  Cyberterrorism is "the use of the computer to commit one or more terrorist acts" (Anderson, Taylor 175).  This can be done in many ways including implanting a computer virus in the enemies computer and destroying all the data saved.  It can also be used to create a "logic bomb" which stays in the system until activated and told to destroy the computer system.
Both the globalization of culture and the increased fear of new forms of terrorism are global concerns that people should be aware of and be educated on.  Knowing about these kinds of sociological issues is so important, especially when it comes to trying to eliminate them.        
      

Chapter 10: Race and Ethnicity

                           


Understanding Bias toward Latinos: Discrimination, 
Dimensions of Difference, and Experience of Exclusion 
John F. Dovidio and Agata Gluszek 
Yale University 
Melissa-Sue John 
University of Connecticut 
Ruth Ditlmann and Paul Lagunes 
Yale University 
White – Black relations have historically been the defining form of intergroup rela- 
tions in the study of prejudice and discrimination. The present article suggests that 
there are limitations to applying this model to understanding bias toward other 
groups and proposes that a comprehensive view of the dynamics of the Anglos’ 
bias toward Latinos requires consideration of the distinctive elements of this form 
of intergroup relations. In four empirical studies, we experimentally document 
discrimination against Latinos (Study 1), explore the potential dimensions that 
underlie bias against Latinos (Study 2), and examine the effect of a particular so- 
cial identity cue, accentedness, on perceptions of acceptance and belongingness 
of Latinos and members of other groups (Studies 3 and 4). These studies consider 
general processes of prejudice and identify how particular facets of bias against 
Latinos can shape their experiences and, taken together, illustrate how under- 
standing bias against Latinos can reciprocally inform contemporary theories of 
prejudice. 
White – Black relations have historically been the defining form of intergroup 
relations in the United States socially, economically, and politically. Jones (1997) 
explained that historically in the United States “blacks have been the largest single 
Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to John F. Dovidio, Depart- 
ment of Psychology, Yale University, P.O. Box 208205, New Haven, CT 06520 [e-mail: john. 
dovidio@yale.edu]. 
Preparation of this article was supported by NSF grant BCS-0613218 awarded to the first author. 
minority group, have been highly salient politically, and have been the focal point 
of legislative, political, judicial, and economic systems. . . . the black experience is 
a paradigm for social justice and policy beliefs in the United States” (pp. 18 – 19). 
Perhaps because of this central role in the U.S. social experience, Black – 
White relations have also been the traditional focus of psychological research 
on prejudice and discrimination, while research on a group that has also ex- 
perienced prejudice and discrimination—Latinos—has been surprisingly rare. 
Ramirez (1988) found that only 7% of the social psychological work on prejudice 
was conducted with Latino targets. Over 20 years later, a review of the articles on 
prejudice and discrimination in three leading social psychology journals (Journal 
of Experimental Social Psychology, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 
and Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin) revealed that the vast majority 
of articles in this area focused on Blacks as targets (61%). Similar to Ramirez 
(1988), only 7% of articles focused on Latinos. 
In addition to dominating the empirical study of prejudice and discrimination, 
Black – White relations have shaped the foundation of theoretical understanding of 
prejudice and bias. Current models of Anglos’ (ethnic majority group members’) 
attitudes toward Latinos and Latino immigrants typically derive directly from 
models of White – Black relations in the United States. Research has generally 
emphasized the role of common underlying processes, such as intergroup threat, 
that contribute to bias across groups (e.g., Z´ 
arate, Garcia, Garza, & Hitlan, 2003). 
Although there is substantial value in emphasizing the common elements 
of bias across groups and promoting general models of intergroup relations, a 
comprehensive view of the dynamics of the attitudes and orientations of Anglos 
toward Latinos requires consideration of the distinctive elements of this form of 
intergroup relations. The current work thus examines the ways that attitudes of 
Anglos toward Latinos and Latino immigrants are similar in nature and dynamics 
to other forms of prejudice but also focuses on additional factors that may be 
particularly relevant for understanding bias toward Latinos. 
Directing more attention to bias toward Latinos is timely, practically as well as 
theoretically. While Black – White relations continue to be of critical importance 
in the United States, the Latino population in the United States has grown in size 
and influence. Latinos have surpassed Blacks as the largest minority group in the 
United States. Latinos currently constitute 15.4% of the U.S. population, whereas 
non-Latino Blacks 12.2% (US Census Bureau, 2009). At the same time, leading 
indicators of economic and social disparities (US Census Bureau, 2007) reveal that 
Latinos, like Blacks, lag behind Whites in earnings (median income based on the 
race of the householder: Blacks, $34,001, Latinos, $40,766, Whites, $55,096) and 
educational attainment for those over 25 years old (BA or higher degree: Blacks, 
17.3%; Latinos, 12.5%; Whites, 30.5%). 
In the current article, in four empirical studies, we examined contempo- 
rary discrimination against Latinos and the experience of Latinos in the United States. Study 1 investigated the operation of bias against Latinos relative to 
Anglos (Whites) in a field setting. Study 2 explored the potential dimensions 
that can underlie bias against Latinos. Studies 3 and 4 examined the effect of a 
particular social identity cue, accentedness, on perceptions of acceptance and be- 
longingness of Latinos and members of other groups. Taken together, these studies 
consider how understanding the general processes of prejudice and bias enhances 
recognition of discrimination against Latinos, as well as how appreciating some 
of the particular facets of bias against Latinos can shape their experiences and 
reciprocally inform contemporary theories of bias. 
Direct empirical evidence of discrimination against Latinos is relatively 
sparse, despite the fact that Latinos represent the largest racial/ethnic minority 
group in the United States, exhibit substantial social and economic disadvantage, 
and report experiences of differential treatment comparable to those reported by 
Blacks (Perez, Fortuna, & Alegria, 2008). We thus first explore evidence of con- 
temporary discrimination again Latinos (Study 1), and then we consider the factors 
that contribute specifically to this type of bias in subsequent studies (Studies 2 – 
4). 
Study 1: Bias toward Latinos 
While there is clear evidence of negative attitudes (prejudice) and stereotypes 
toward Latinos (Deaux & Ethier, 1998), documentation of discriminatory behavior 
is rare. Archival evidence often implicates discrimination against Latinos, for ex- 
ample in earnings of Latino-owned firms (Carvajal, 2004) or in capital sentencing 
(Lee, 2007). Experimental research has revealed discrimination against Latinos in 
simulated jury decision-making contexts (Bottoms, Davis, & Epstein, 2004) and 
when applying for low-wage jobs (Pager, 2007). However, other studies reveal 
that Latinos are evaluated comparably to Anglos for equivalent performance (e.g., 
Romero & Garza, 1986). 
One explanation for the inconsistent evidence of bias against Latinos in lab- 
oratory studies is that participants are often concerned about revealing their true 
underlying prejudice (Dovidio & Gaertner, 2004). Consistent with this reasoning, 
discrimination against Latinos is more likely to be observed when participants 
are unaware that their social biases are being examined and/or the context offers 
justifications other than ethnicity for responding negatively. In Study 1, we thus 
examined potential discrimination against Latinos in a field experiment (Ditlmann 
& Lagunes, 2009). 
In particular, Study 1 investigated differential treatment of Latino shoppers. 
We recorded the responses of cashiers to requests by one of three Anglo and three 
Latino male confederates to purchase a gift certificate.
Method 
Participants. The participants were cashiers in 217 stores in four different 
locations in Connecticut. Latino confederates visited 111 stores and Anglo con- 
federates visited 106. The stores all indicated over the telephone that they sold 
$10 gift certificates and accepted personal checks as a valid form of payment. 
Procedure. Three male Latino and three male Anglo confederates, matched 
on five key characteristics (age, weight, attractiveness, extroversion, and mas- 
culinity), were selected from a pool of 40 Latino and Anglo men on the basis 
of three raters’ agreement that they would be readily perceived as Latino or 
Anglo. These confederates received extensive training that involved memorizing 
and practicing the script through role-play and at actual stores, and standardizing 
confederates’ appearances and verbal and nonverbal behaviors. To keep confed- 
erates unaware of the purpose of the study, confederates were separated during 
training and the execution of the study. None suspected the true purpose of the 
research. In addition, 34 of the 217 interactions in the stores were monitored by a 
trained observer without the knowledge of the confederates. The observers found 
that the confederate followed the protocol in all cases. 
The confederates, who were randomly assigned to stores, first requested to 
purchase a $10 gift certificate. If the salesperson responded that the minimum 
dollar amount was larger than $10, the confederate was instructed to accept this 
higher limit. The confederate then attempted to pay with a personal check. The 
main measures of bias against Latinos were (a) the minimum dollar amount 
confederates were quoted for the gift certificate, and (b) the frequency with which 
Latino confederates, relative to Anglo confederates, were asked for identification 
when they paid with a personal check. 
Results 
Overall, Latino confederates were treated differently than Anglo confederates 
on both measures of bias. Despite the fact that all of the stores included in the study 
offered $10 gift certificates, logistical regression analysis revealed that salespeople 
quoted a minimum dollar amount higher than $10 more frequently for Latino than 
for White confederates, 9% versus 2% of the time, b 
= 1.64, SE = .78, p < .05. 
Salespeople also asked for identification more frequently when the confederate 
was Latino than Anglo, 85% versus 73%, b 
0.69, SE .34, p < .05. 
Discussion 
Taken together these findings demonstrated that salespeople treated Latino 
confederates differently and less positively than they treated Anglo confederates.
Thus, paralleling the patterns of disparities in major social indicators (e.g., in- 
come) and perceptions of experiencing discrimination (Perez et al., 2008), Latinos 
may experience discrimination today much as Blacks do. The nature of these 
contemporary biases is relatively subtle; Latino confederates were not openly re- 
fused service nor were they the target of blatantly racist remarks by salespeople. 
Nevertheless, with respect to racial discrimination, Dovidio and Gaertner (2004) 
noted that subtle expressions of bias can have negative consequences as insidious 
as blatant discrimination. Moreover, microaggressions of this type can be particu- 
larly cognitively and emotionally taxing for members of stigmatized groups (Sue 
et al., 2007; see also Quintana, Herrera, & Nelson, this issue). Thus, even slight 
but consistent disparities in treatment can have a cumulative negative effect for 
Latinos in terms of psychological well-being (e.g., Finch, Kolody, & Vega, 2000), 
social adjustment (e.g., Greene, Way, & Pahl, 2006), and aspirations (e.g., Devos 
& Torres, 2007) over time. 
While Study 1 suggests parallels in the discriminatory treatment of Blacks and 
Latinos and its consequences, we note that the underlying dynamics of discrimi- 
nation that lead to actual and perceived exclusion may differ somewhat between 
the groups. The nature of Whites’ attitudes toward Blacks and their intergroup 
relations have been shaped by the legacy of slavery and the historical relationships 
between Blacks and Whites throughout the history of the United States (see Jones, 
1997). Black Americans generally share common language, history, and political 
destiny with White Americans. By contrast, the Latino population has shown dra- 
matic growth in the United States relatively recently, and it is culturally diverse, 
reflecting different histories than that of Anglo Americans. 
In addition, recent concerns about illegal immigration can play a critical role 
in the attitudes of Anglos toward Latinos in general (see also Z´ 
arate & Shaw, 
this issue). Latinos constitute 80% of the estimated 11.6 million undocumented 
immigrants in the United States who have been the focus of considerable de- 
bate and criticism (Deaux, 2004; Hoefer, Rytina, & Baker, 2009). In 2007, the 
issue of illegal immigration continued to be of “great importance” or “very im- 
portant” for 62% of respondents in a Gallup survey (Carroll, 2007), and 43% of 
respondents believed that the United States had lost ground with regard to ille- 
gal immigration (Saad, 2007). Substantial portions of the U.S. population blame 
undocumented immigrants for taking jobs away from Americans, suppressing 
wages, contributing to higher unemployment, and imposing a fiscal burden on 
other tax payers (Deaux, 2006). It is likely that negative attitudes toward un- 
documented immigrants extend to Latino immigrants in general because of how 
closely associated these two groups are. Thus, bias toward Latinos may involve 
an additional dimension, one of foreignness, compared to bias against Blacks. 
Our next study represented our initial exploration of the different dimensions 
that may underlie prejudice and discrimination against different racial and ethnic 
groups. 
Study 2: Dimensions of Difference 
The United States is a pluralist society composed of many identifiable ethnic 
groups. However, despite demographic trends producing unprecedented racial and 
ethnic diversity, the standard for being American remains basically singular— 
being White (Devos & Banaji, 2005; Sidanius, Feshbach, Levin, & Pratto, 1997). 
However, the distinction between which groups are seen as American is not a 
simple in-group – out-group perception. Various minority groups in the United 
States may differ systematically in how “American” they are perceived to be by 
Whites. Devos and Banaji (2005), for example, found that people were more likely 
to pair White with American than either Asian or Black with American on implicit 
tasks, thus demonstrating that White is the prototypical image of American. Using 
a direct measure of similarity of various groups of citizens to Americans (1 
= 
disagree strongly to 7 
= agree strongly) in a preliminary study, we found that 
White Americans were judged most similar to—virtually synonymous with— 
Americans (M 
= 6.69). Black Americans were rated next most similar (M = 
6.17) but significantly lower than White Americans; they were followed by Latino 
Americans (M 
= 5.88), Asian Americans (M = 5.79), Native Americans (M = 
5.67), and Arab Americans (M 
= 4.94). Study 2 therefore examined the different 
“shades” of being perceived as American for different racial/ethnic groups and 
explored the dimensionsthatdistinguishthesegroups. Despite increasing pluralism in the United States, as we observed earlier, 
models of intergroup relations have focused primarily on Black – White relations. 
Nevertheless, the underlying causes of prejudice and discrimination may differ. 
Social groups have different histories and varying levels of contact, power, and 
status; these variables play an important role in how people are categorized and 
stereotyped. Our research thus focused on determinants of how people respond to 
members within an inclusive ingroup category, Americans. Studying who is con- 
sidered American and how that judgment is determined can enhance understanding 
of both the unique and common influences that shape bias toward members of 
various groups, including Latinos. 
Much of the previous research on the extent to which members of different 
groups are perceived to be American has focused on a single dimension of differ- 
ence (Devos & Banaji, 2005; Sidanius et al., 1997), but recent research suggests 
that more than one dimension may be involved in national definitions. In partic- 
ular, Pehrson, Vignoles, and Brown (2009) identified two dimensions of national 
inclusion, ethnic nationalism and civic nationalism. Ethnic nationalism defines 
national inclusion in terms of shared ancestry, often reflected by physical appear- 
ance and language as well as by formal heritage. From this perspective, group 
membership has a strong essentialist quality, in which ethnic as well as racial 
groups are often differentiated along a “biological” continuum (Jost & Hamilton, 
2005). For example, until recently Germany relied on a model of citizenship that
Bias toward Latinos 65 
was based primarily on jus sanguinis (right of blood), in which citizenship was 
restricted to the natural offspring of citizens. Civic nationalism, in contrast, defines 
national group membership in terms of commitment to the ideals and standards 
perceived to define a nation. Ideology rather than biology represents the defining 
basis for national inclusion. 
Although these dimensions are sometimes perceived as oppositional, more 
recently they have been conceptualized as separate, independent dimensions un- 
derlying nationalistic expressions (Pehrson et al., 2009). Indeed, Devos and Banaji 
(2005, Study 1) found that items related to ethnic identity/nativity, such as “being 
born in America” and “having lived in America most of one’s life,” and items 
associated with civic orientation, such as “respect America’s political institutions 
and laws” and “be patriotic,” loaded on different dimensions in a factor analysis 
of items defining American identity. 
Thus, within the context of relations between two groups, such as Blacks 
and Whites, only the single dimension of “distance” may be relevant. However, 
when multiple group relations are considered more dimensions can emerge. The 
goal of Study 2 was to explore different dimensions that determine how various 
groups deviate from the American prototype. This study used college students as 
participants for comparability to previous work (e.g., Devos & Banaji, 2005) and 
to assess a newer generation’s intergroup perceptions. These data were collected 
as part of a larger project on intergroup perceptions (John, 2009). We hypothesized 
that these two dimensions, ethnic and civic, define not only how much different 
groups are seen as American but also the fundamental ways in which these groups 
differ from the American prototype. That is, we propose that the distance from the 
American prototype varies in two-dimensional space reflecting ethnic and civic 
(ideological) dimensions. For instance, Black Americans are likely to differ more 
from White Americans (who are expected to most closely represent the prototype 
of Americans) than would Latino Americans on the ethnic dimension. However, 
because of their long history in the United States with Whites, Black Americans 
would be expected to diverge less than Latinos on the civic dimension. 
Method 
Participants. Sixty White college students (31 men and 29 women) between 
the ages of 18 and 22 participated in the study for course credit. 
Procedure. Participants completed a survey in which they were asked to 
indicate the similarity of 22 social groups with each other, on a 1 (disagree strongly) 
to 7 (agree strongly) scale. Groups of primary interest included American, White 
American, Black American, Latino American, Asian American, Native American, 
and Arab American.
Results 
Multidimensional scaling, ageometricdatamappingtechniquefordatathat 
expressthedistancesamongtheobjectsofaset,wasusedtoanalyzethesimilarity 
ratings. Evaluationofthescreeplotindicatedthatthereweretwoprimaryunder- 
lyingdimensions. Figure1showsthesevengroupsof primaryinterest mapped 
ontothistwo-dimensional space. Thehorizontal axisappearstoreflect acivic 
dimension, whereastheverticalaxisrepresentsanethnicdimension. 
Ashypothesized,andconsistentwiththefindingsofDevosandBanaji(2005) 
andSidaniusetal. (1997), WhiteAmericansappearmostprototypicalofAmeri- 
cans;theyarethegroupclosesttoAmericaninthistwo-dimensionalspace.Asian 
AmericansandBlackAmericansbothdifferedsubstantially(but indifferent di- 
rections)fromWhiteAmericansontheethnicitydimension,butBlackAmericans 
werecloseronthecivicdimension. Ofallofthesegroups, ArabAmericanswere 
furthest awayfromAmericansandWhiteAmericansonthecivicdimension. Of 
particularrelevancetothefocusofthepresentarticle,LatinoAmericansdeviated 
modestlyfromWhiteAmericans(andBlackAmericans)onthecivicdimension 
butsubstantiallyfromWhiteAmericansontheethnicdimension.Thus,whilethe 
absolutedistancesnicelyreplicatedpreviousresearchonthedifferent degreesto 
which various groups reflect “Americanness,” this study extended the previous 
work by identifying two fundamental dimensions that determine how much and 
in what ways different groups deviate from the American prototype. 
Discussion 
We believe that these findings have important implications for understanding 
the ways that different groups may be perceived and treated, as well for how 
different minority groups may experience stigma. With respect to differential 
treatment, the nature of bias expressed toward Blacks, Latinos, Asians, Arabs, 
and Native Americans may vary systematically as a function of how these groups 
deviate from Whites on the ethnic and civic dimensions. When groups differ from 
Whites on dimensions related to ethnicity, Whites may be particularly likely to 
attribute the negative actions of members of these groups to negative dispositional 
characteristics, reflecting the essentialist quality of this dimension (e.g., Hewstone, 
1990). Because people make stronger dispositional attributions for more novel 
and distinctive stimuli, our analysis further suggests that this type of intergroup 
bias may be more pronounced for groups perceived to deviate more from White 
Americans on the ethnic dimension (e.g., more for Blacks than for Latinos). 
Distances from White Americans on the civic dimension may also system- 
atically affect the degree and nature of intergroup bias (see also Ryan, Casas, 
& Thompson, this issue). With respect to the degree of bias, when Whites per- 
ceive that an action violates conventional standards (e.g., a crime is committed), 
Whites may be more sensitive to detecting these violations and may respond more 
negatively to members of groups more distant from White Americans on this di- 
mension; these groups are already perceived to deviate more from American moral 
standards. Thus, Whites may punish members of different groups more severely 
for moral violations as a function of the group’s distance from White Americans 
or the American prototype on this dimension. Future research might consider 
whether individual differences in the extent to which Anglos associate Latinos 
with illegal immigration increase their perception of the distance of Latinos from 
White Americans on the civic dimension, and how this distance relates to the 
treatment of individual Latinos and support for policies that benefit the group as 
a whole. 
Moreover, people may respond in qualitatively different ways to violations 
of standards by members of groups viewed as close to or distant from White 
Americans on the civic dimension. Specifically, whereas punishment may be 
direct and corrective for close groups (e.g., Blacks), people may instead choose to 
isolate and ostracize members of other groups who are perceived to deviate from 
the civic standards of prototypical Americans. For instance, people may be more 
likely to recommend deportation of a Latino American than a Black American for 
committing a crime, even though both are American citizens and thus cannot be
legally deported. Thus, a two-dimensional perspective on group relations suggests 
some novel directions for future research on discrimination. 
Members of minority groups are also vigilant to differential treatment by 
majority group members and fear rejection associated with stigmatization of their 
group (Mendoza-Denton, Downey, Purdie, Davis, & Pietrzak, 2002). Thus, minor- 
ity group members may be particularly sensitive to behaving in ways that increase 
the salience of how their group might differ from the American prototype on 
the civic or ethnic dimension. With respect to the civic dimension, for example, 
minority group members who feel socially vulnerable in a majority group con- 
text may affirm conventional, majority-group standards even more emphatically 
than majority group members. Cues of deviance along the ethnic dimension, such 
as comments related to physical difference, may similarly lead minority group 
members to emphasize the ways that they are similar to the American prototypic 
standards (Cheryan & Monin, 2005). In the next study, we investigated one impli- 
cation of this analysis. Specifically, we examined how self-perceptions of accents, 
which represent an indicator of difference or foreignness, relate to feelings of 
belongingness and social acceptance among members of different groups. 
Whereas Study 2 suggested a broader framework for studying intergroup re- 
lations beyond the traditional Black – White paradigm, Studies 3 and 4 focused on 
one aspect of that framework. Ethnic difference defined one of the two fundamen- 
tal dimensions determining where different groups were situated relative to the 
American prototype. Studies 3 and 4 thus considered the relationship of a com- 
mon cue of ethnic difference, possessing a nonnative accent, with how members of 
various groups, including Latinos, feel included or excluded in the United States. 
Specifically, the goal of Studies 3 and 4 was to examine the association between 
participants’ perceptions of their own accents and social belonging (Study 3) and 
perceived discrimination (Study 4). Study 3 explored the responses of people with 
nonnative accents generally; Study 4 included enough participants with nonnative 
accents to examine the responses of Latinos, Asians, and Europeans separately. 
The data for these two studies represent a subset of data that are being collected 
for a larger project (Gluszek & Dovidio, in press). 

Other Websites on the Topic: 

http://www.trinity.edu/mKearl/race.html  The topic of this website is race and ethnicity.  It explains the history of immigration and lists some of the different racial and ethnic groups that live in the United States.  It also provides links to other websites that directly relate to the specific racial and ethnic groups that have immigrated to America.  

http://www.globalissues.org/article/165/racism America is not the only place that racism occurs and this website explains the types of racism that happen all over the world in many different countries.  It is an extremely informative website that also provides links to other websites regarding the same topic.  

                                                                                  

                     stereotypes of blacks, mexicans, asians, gays, and blondes


                                         

                                         Word Search

E
T
H
N
I
C
G
R
O
U
P
N
E
R
T
I
K
C
I
Q
T
I
O
L
F
D
A
R
C
W
E
R
T
U
A
O
B
E
F
B
W
D
G
S
E
G
T
W
B
M
B
R
E
A
E
Y
I
P
W
Q
F
J
H
F
S
A
V
G
W
P
Z
Y
T
W
P
O
W
N
U
I
O
E
N
R
D
E
L
R
Y
O
P
U
I
W
S
D
C
B
U
T
I
T
R
U
I
D
I
S
C
R
I
M
I
N
A
T
I
O
N
T
R
A
S
I
M
E
A
R
T
C
U
O
I
S
A
R
G
A
S
C
E
R
T
C
I
O
E
V
B
Y
E
I
E
F
L
T
F
G
M
N
I
T
I
A
L
P
Q
M
E
W
S
I
I
W
T
A
S
S
I
M
I
L
A
T
I
O
N
A
S
O
E
T
N
B
M
A
E
T
R
L
A
T
I
E
T
M
V
A
S
R
E
Y
I
O
P
K
L
G
I
S
T
S
U
B
I
N
O
I
T
A
G
E
R
G
E
S
N
G
G
S
N
R
Y
I
O
P
B
C
R
G
J
H
M
A
A
J
A
D
G
T
J
U
I
E
W
S
F
H
Y
J
K
O
A
F
F
I
R
M
A
T
I
V
E
A
C
T
I
O
N

Ethnic group: A social category of people who share a common culture, for example, a common language or dialect; a common nationality; a common religion; and common norms, practices, customs and history.
Race: A group treated as distinct in society based on certain characteristics, some of which are biological, that have been assigned social importance.
Prejudice: Evaluation of a social group and the individuals within it based on conceptions about the social group held despite facts that disprove them; the beliefs involve both prejudgment and misjudgment.
Discrimination: Overt negative and unequal treatment of the members of some social group or stratum solely because of their membership in that group or stratum.
Racism: The perception and treatment of a racial or ethnic group, or member of that group, as intellectually, socially, and culturally inferior to one’s own group.
Anti-Semitism: The hatred of Jewish people.
Segregation: The spatial and social separation of racial and ethnic groups.
Assimilation: Process by which a minority becomes socially, economically, and culturally absorbed within the dominant society. 
Pluralism: The maintenance and persistence of one’s culture, language, mannerisms, practices, art and so on.  
Affirmative Action: A heavily contested program for change, is a race-specific policy for reducing job and educational inequality that has had some limited success. 

RACISM: Song lyrics
I can't believe what they did
what I saw on T.V.
in the 20th century
They beat a man like a dog
to the ground, yeah, they made him bleed
what does it mean
I just want to tell the world to stop 
all the hating wars
it's only tearing us apart
If we're going to live together on this earth
We need to learn to stop Racism
On the news everyday
someone blows someone away
It's all because of hate
People persecute and fear,
discriminate, what is our fate
Iran, Iraq,Bosnia, South Africa the IRA
to name a few today
So much to hate,worth killing for
Your colored skin, your God and more
what is this for 
Tim Lamberson 1994


Chapter 10: Race and Ethnicity 

I chose to make this post on chapter ten because I feel that the prejudices and stereotypes towards minority, racial and ethnic groups in the United States need to come to an end.  I also feel that one of the best ways to combat this is by educating people on the matter.  This is why I decided to talk about racism,  racial stereotypes, and prejudice and discrimination.  

The picture at the beginning of the post shows children from all over the world standing together.  Each of them come from very different backgrounds and are all part of different ethnic groups.  The picture symbolizes the idea that it doesn't matter what ethnic or racial group you are a part of; we are all equal.  The article just beneath the picture focusses on the discrimination against latinos in the United States.  I specifically chose this article because chapter ten discusses Latino Americans and how they came to immigrate to the United States and I wanted to give readers a better understanding of some of the ways they are often discriminated against.  The article also includes the results of experiments that have been conducted on behalf of Latinos and how they are often treated in certain situations.  The next thing I included in my post are the two links to websites that are related to the topics found in chapter ten.  The first one is a website regarding the different ethnic groups in the United States. I chose to include this website because it does a wonderful job in providing information on the history of the groups and provides links to other sites that go into even greater detail on the discrimination they often face.  I think people often forget that prejudice and discrimination towards specific ethnic and racial groups happens all over the world in all different countries, not just in the U.S. That is why I included this website. It gives examples of the prejudices that occur in different countries.  The fourth thing I included in my post is a video that gives examples of racial stereotypes.  I found this video interesting because it showed people of different races and beliefs talking about how they are often stereotyped.  The way the video was set up allowed the viewer to see how real and painful these stereotypes are to them.  Underneath the video is the word-search filled with words that represent the ideas found in chapter ten the most accurately.  Lastly, I chose to incorporate the song "Racism" because it represents the feelings that a lot of people have towards racism.  There are a lot of people out there who want racism and the discrimination of others to come to an end.